3. Behavioral Evolution II

3.2M views16407 WordsCopy TextShare
Stanford
(April 2, 2010) Robert Sapolsky continues his two-part series on evolution focusing on individual an...
Video Transcript:
[Music] Stanford University. Okay, let's get going. Various announcements, procedural things. Uh, number of people want more information about grading and what the exams are like, all of that. I think I mentioned one-third of the points will come from the midterm, twothirds from the final. Uh, in terms of the style of the midterm, the midterm is heavily going to be about making sure you got down all the factoids from the first half of the course, that you've got the basics of each of our proverbial buckets. The second half, the final is all going to be about
integration, thinking across the different categories. So just the sense of that readings readings as they are coming out the books are not required until the second half of the course. The handout on Monday I think said which chapters of the zebra book you should read. Uh we will shortly get to you which chapters of the chaos book uh you should read as well. Uh the readings that are being posted on the courseworks the downloads of various published papers those are required. I'm clear on those whether this is a paper you should read all of. This
is one you should read the abstract of. Even if you read all of, do not read it in some obsessive detailoriented way. The goal is probably to be able to say in one or two paragraphs why this paper has something pertinent to say about the topic they fell into. You're not sitting there having to memorize techniques, uh, middle names of the authors, how many animals, anything like that. Um, in terms of that, it probably makes sense to read those after the first lecture of whatever block there is. And hopefully, if I get organized, I'll be
able to get you uh sort of the list of the readings further in advance than one week in advance. Nonetheless, you should probably hold off reading it until after the lecture occurs. Let's see what else. People wanted to get a sense of how long things were going to go. And as we'll see today, the evolution lecture topic will cover two classes. Molecular genetics, which is what we'll pick up on Monday, I'm guessing one to one and a half. Behavior genetics following that one to one and a half, ethology, one neurobiology, endocrinology, we'll have one week
devoted to intro to the topics. And again, that's one where this is so important for everybody to be up to speed rather than these being in catchup sections that week. The whole week, Monday, Wednesday, Friday, will be devoted to that with the TAs teaching it. The following week, three more lectures, more advanced ones. And depending on proximity to the midterm, there may be a half lecture in there on statistics or maybe not. So, it's going to depend on keeping on schedule. This is a rough approximation. The midterm is going to be a Monday night. uh
you will be responsible for material up to the previous Wednesday and there'll be lots of review stuff. Take a look at the extended notes being posted. What else? Um okay, I think that covers most of the procedural stuff. All of the stuff will get posted as well. So picking up on the other day, what was happening the other day? Number one, the trouncing of Darwin inventing evolution. Trouncing of survival of the fittest. probably most importantly trouncing of behavior for the good of the species group selection type arguments. What we saw was number one the rationale
for the whole thing. There is a vicious unfightable logic to why hearts have to be the size they are and kidneys have the filtration rates they have to have in order to solve the challenges of leaving as many copies of your genes in the next generation. and making sense of the evolution of hearts and kidneys and things like that could be the worlds of bioengineers and biomechanics folks with an underlying logic that it's got to be something that increases the number of copies of genes that you leave. And the whole rationale for Wednesday's lecture and
today is applying the same sort of logic to behavior. the whole world of just as you can optimize sort of the way one's neck how long it is if you're a giraffe you can optimize behavioral strategies and again also throwing in a caveat no animal is sitting there maybe with the exception of some other apes sitting there consciously strategizing along those lines when saying so what would you as this dandelion want to do at that point with this ecological challenge personifying just to make things easier Okay, what we've then barreled into were the three major
building blocks for thinking about the evolution of behavior in the framework of contemporary evolutionary thinking. Number one, individual selection. Passing on as many copies of your own gene to the next generation as possible by way of your own reproducing. The individual selection, a chicken is an egg's way to make another egg. I've now rehearsed that. So, I've got that down right. the whole notion of behavior as just being this epipen phenomenon in order to do what's needed to get another copy of the genes into the next generation. Building block number two, some of the time
the best way to increase the number of genes you pass on to the next generation is to help your relatives do so. following that logic of mandelian relatedness and people in the catchup section I know went over issues of why is it that you share half your genes with a full sibling a quarter with a half sibling etc. So sometimes the way to maximize is by helping out a relative to do so with again constrained by this vicious mathematical logic of it depends on how related you are to the relative and thus you will gladly
lay down your life for one identical twin, two full siblings, eight cousins, off you go. So the whole notion there of insight into why social animals the galaxy over are so obsessed with kinship and relatedness the whole world of who counts as an us who counts as a them in terms of cooperative behaviors playing out along lines of relatedness. Finally we saw the third piece which was reciprocal altruism. You scratch my back, I'll scratch yours. Many hands make the task less scratchy or whatever. And what you see in those cases there is a whole world
in which you don't have to be related to have cooperation. And we saw all the domains of that bringing in the formalization biomechanics person figuring out how strong a leg bone has to be a game theorist figuring out within the realm of social behavior when you cooperate when you don't what sort of strategies. Game theory of seeing the prisoners dilemma as the building block of that entire field. and seeing all the strategies worked out by mathematicians and economists and diplomats and seeing which ones optimize under what circumstance and then going and looking in the real
world and seeing there's all sorts of animals out there that have evolved optimization strategies of when to cooperate and when to defect. And what we'll get to as a huge huge issue by the time we get to the lectures on aggression, cooperation, etc. is well that's great when you've got a cooperative system going. How do they ever start? How do you jumpstart cooperation in systems the evolution of cooperation? That's something we will get to in great detail down the line. Finally, what we shifted to was now saying great, we've got all these principles in hand
here and our big three building blocks and all of that. How would it be applied to making sense of animal behavior out in the real world? And we left with the example starting someplace there. Where did it go? Yes, that's it. Of us marching through. No one individual factoid about some species or other species. Know that there's a big difference between the genders and size or there isn't. Know that there's not high levels of aggression in males. Know that females always give birth. Whatever those traits are, as we marched through using these ideas about individual
selection, kin selection, reciprocal altruism, you could march through and logically infer what the social behavior of this particular species was going to be like. And you would be right. We saw, for example, in tournament species, tournament species where you have high levels of aggression among males, malemale competition for access to females. As a result, males tend to be a lot bigger than females. They are being selected for muscle mass, secondary sexual characteristics, plumage, big shark canines for slashing the other guy. Bringing up this issue of in a tournament species, who does the male want to
mate with? What sort of female are males interested in mating with? The answer being anyone who will mate with them because there's like no cost involved. All that's involved is no parental behavior, none of that. the cost of sperm and literally people analyze the relative cost of sperm versus eggs. In tournament species, males are dramatically unchoosy as to who they mate with. In tournament species, males have dramatic variability in reproductive success. 5% accounting for 95% of the mings. Female choice. In a tournament species, what does a female want out of a male? She certainly is
not going to get good fatherhood out of the guy. All she wants are good genes because that's all she could hope for from the guy. A whole world of female selectivity for markers of good genes. And as we'll see in the sexual behavioral lectures, a whole world of males trying to fake out females across the animal world suggesting they got better genes than they actually do. So we will come to that. Then tournament species. Do you see female abandonment of kids? Absolutely not. Because there's nobody else who's going to take care of them. Coupled with
that, you see single births among those species, low level, high levels of aggression amongst the males, low levels of parenting, and males tend to live a lot shorter than females. And what we saw at the other end, the pair bonded species, was a completely different picture. Males are being careful who they're mating with because the wiring there is you mate, you take care of the kids. High levels of male parenting. Females are thus able to abandon them. Females can have multiple births. Males are selected to be as close to females as a male can be.
So, there's not a big difference in size. There's not a big difference in secondary sexual characteristics. There's not high levels of aggression. There's not big differences in lifespan. Who does a female want to mate with? Someone who is going to be a competent father. Thus, the whole world of pair bonded species where males court with rituals of coming up with food and coming up with things of that sort. So the most striking thing about that was again going back to the two skulls I brought in. You could know nothing more about that species other than
this is an adult female. This is an adult male and working through the same logic. You know a huge amount about the private lives of this species and who's messing around with who in the bushes. And that's merely by applying these principles. and likewise saying here's a picture of a pair in this species and you can't tell which one is who by gender that tells you a whole world of predictability at the other end. So a first example of how much explanatory power you can get out of this and on the website I will find
all sorts of appropriate pictures of tournament and pair bonding species for you to look at. Okay. to a first realm of looking at animal behavior and seeing how once we got these principles in hand, individual selection, kin selection, reciprocal altruism, suddenly an awful lot of stuff makes sense. Next domain where that's the case and this is one that has had a real challenge to the ooh isn't nature benign and animals behavior they go to the species viewpoint. This one domain probably proves an individual selection framework more than anything else out there. Okay, back to our
inevitable National Geographic specials. And somewhere at the end of whatever the special is about whatever species at some point they usually get some narrator with some deep baritone voice come in at that stage and say something like, "Man is the only species that kills for pleasure. Man is the only and look at how wonderful and benign these rose bushes are. They don't kill. They don't have war. Only humans do." And what occurred by about the mid70s or so was enough field workers from a different species reporting, hey, wait a second, we're not the only species
that kills. And we will see plenty of domains where that makes sense in the lectures to come. But one particular version really really demanded some rethinking about animal behavior, which was as follows. What is one of the universals? Whether you start with us and go all the way down to slime molds, it's babies are cute. Everybody likes babies. Babies are adorable. You want to take care of babies. Your eyes dilate as soon as you're around them. And a longstanding notion that what infants, what babies, what baby features are about or among other things are means
to reduce aggression. And we will see in the ethology lectures something about that. That was the standard soundbite forever. And suddenly you got field biologists coming back from studying various species saying something's up there because in my species um I see individuals murdering kids all the time. Whoa. What's up with that infanticide within a species? Suddenly, not only are humans not the only species that kills, we're like not the only one who goes and kills adorable little Disney infants. Suddenly all sorts of species popping up where there was infanticide and the huge challenge then became
to make sense of this. This first emerged in the 1970s studies of langanger monkeys in India by someone named Sarah Herurie reporting this. Soon it was reported among lions some other species as well. And the first obvious response to this was wait this can't be because I watch all the wildlife specials. There's something growing. Oh there's some sort of psychopathology going on. This is not a normal population. This is not a normal population because they live close to humans. There's some disturbance. There's a toxic waste dump somewhere. They're not a normal species because you've got
the wrong color socks on as the observer. Whatever it is, this is not normal. This is pathological behavior. But long enough time goes by and it becomes clear in all sorts of species, individuals kill infants. So what is this about? You start to look closely and there's patterns. There are patterns to it. The first one being that uh tends to be adult males who kill infants. The next being you look closely and it's not random who kills who. It's males killing infants who are most likely to be the offspring of, you guessed it, individual selection,
most likely to be the offspring of other males. competitive strategies for reducing some other guy's reproductive success. So this began to be seen in these species that had infanticide. It was male infanticide of offspring who were most like of kids who were most likely to be the offspring of other males. But more patterns popped up at that time which is well why don't you see that in any social species where you have competition? And what wound up being clear after a while is there's only a certain pattern that you see in species that have competitive
infanticide, which is the average interbirth interval among females is longer than the average tenure of a high-ranking male. What? Here's what that means. That means you're some lowranking guy. You've been working out in the gym for years on end. You're finally in a position to boot out the alpha male and take over the whole group. And damn, every single female in the group has a newborn who they're going to be nursing for the next 2 and 1/2 years. So, they're not going to be ovulating and they're not going to be ovulating for about 3 years.
And you're likely to be alpha for only a year and a half or so, where the length of time on average that you are going to be able to be reproductively active is shorter than the length of time that females in your species nurse kids and thus are not ovulating. And suddenly you have this brutally clear logic that makes perfect sense from everything on Monday, which is go and kill the kids. Go and kill the kids for two reasons. Number one, by killing the offspring of some other male, you are decreasing that individual's reproductive success.
This competition is leaving as many copies of your genes, etc. Number two, by killing an offspring, the female by stopping nursing will soon be ovulating. And thus you see this pattern in langanger monkeys, vervet monkeys, pattis monkeys, lions, uh mountain gorillas and such where it's always this structure, competitive infanticide. A male takes over a breeding group and goes about systematically trying to kill the infants. So we've got an enormous violation there of behaving for the good of the species and baby animals are cute and suppress aggression, all that sort of thing. The clearest demonstration of
that is to look at the fact that one of the species where this goes on by this this vicious sort of logic of competition. This is one of the species on Earth that is as close to extinction as you can get. Mountain gorillas. There's maybe 6 700 of them are left and these are just like stunning animals and they are disappearing because of habitat degradation and human presence and civil wars sweeping through there and all that sort of stuff. But also they are facing I think inevitable extinction in a decade or two because every now
and then a male mountain gorilla will systematically go and kill the infants around in a group by this logic. You can't ask for a much better demonstration of animals not behaving for the good of the species or the group this individual selection strategy. So immediately we start applying some rules to this. Well, when's a circumstance that a male would come into a group and boot out the previous reproductive male and take over and where he wouldn't be all that fast to try to kill all the infants in the group? When would a male take over
a group and not be all that thrilled to do this competitive infanticide strategy? Any guesses? A brother. Yeah. Part two. Individual selection. Kin selection. Kin selection. you don't see the competitive infanticide if the males replacing are close relatives by exactly that logic. So then you begin to see some more elegant stuff going on. In some cases in a bunch of species instead of males killing infants what happens is the presence of a new male causes females to miscarry pregnant females to misaryry. One version of this and this is seen in wild horses. the new male
in there harasses pregnant females to the point of them miscarrying. Same exact logic again in a whole bunch of rodent species. You see something far more elegant. The new male shows up and if you were a pregnant female, the smell of a new male causes you to misarry a litter. And the biology of that is completely worked out. Going from the olfactory signaling, and we're going to hear tons about olfactory signaling in weeks to come. Going from the olfactory system to release of a stress hormone that disrupts uterine maturation and you abort, olfaction induced abortion.
What's up with the female with that? Why is that a strategy for her to leave as many copies of her own genes in the next generation? If this guy is around, she's got a choice at that point. a choice. Again, metaphorically, she has a choice at that point. Either she can go through the rest of the metabolic costs of pregnancy, give birth to these kids, and then they get killed, or at least she can take the lesser of two evils. He spontaneously aborts at that point. And soon after, she is ovulating and has a chance
again to pass on copies of her own genes. And this is the whole world of you put a male hamster in with a female who's just had babies and he goes and eats the kids. Oh, it's psychopathological. It's not psychopathological. Male hamsters are migratory. If there are kids there, they're not likely to have been mine. That's the logical thing. Females having been selected for if there is the smell of a new male miscarry. And the same logic again extending the smell of a new male unless he's a close relative of the previous male. You see
the exact same thing playing out there. Now all of this of course is very sort of male biased in terms of this. These are females sort of making the best of the bad situation. You're some female langanger monkey and some new male has come in and is intent on being infanticidal and you've read evolutionary biology also. What do you want to do to maximize copies of your own genes in the next generation? Well, you've got this kid here who you're trying to protect. Uh, would you protect that child to the point of your being killed?
Probably not. And you don't see females doing that. Under what circumstance would a female be most motivated to defend her child to the point of her being seriously injured? Older females. Older females who are less likely to have another child after that. You're on the scene there. And who else from day one would be most willing to get injured in order to protect the kid? the maternal grandmother who has no reproductive potential at that point. And what you see in these lang monkeys, the females will defend not to the point of serious injury. The older
the mother is, the more strenuously she defends and the grandmothers defend even more following the same logic here playing out in that way. But you also have another option which is to do something much more clever. And this is not in the world of after you've given birth, but the world of when you are pregnant. Because even in these species that have the competitive infanticide, the new male is also perfectly happy to harass a pregnant female. What have females evolved in a number of primate species? One of the great all-time ways of manipulating males. So,
you're this pregnant langanger monkey and in comes this new male and you know you're going to go through like six more weeks of pregnancy and they're going to be enormously costly and the damn guy is going to kill your kids as soon as they're born and what can you do and all of that and you come up with something very clever physiologically. You go into what is called pseudoestrus. estrus being the term for when female primates are ovulating or sexually receptive are in heat and most primate species give external signs of when they're ovulating. There's
engorgment of all sorts of tissues around the vagina and rear end and stuff and that humans don't do but they have some of the physiological residues of water retention for example. So you've got all these external signs of I am ovulating except you're not ovulating because you're pregnant. You are pseudo ovulating. you were going through a pseudo estress, you generate the picture of it. So, what happens at that point with this new guy who's just shown up and here's this like four-month pregnant female who nonetheless looks like she's going through estrus. Well, these guys sit
there then and say, "Well, well, that's kind of nice." And they go and mate with her and you know, like 2 and 1/2 weeks later, she gives birth. And you know what the guy's going to do? He's going to sit there and say, "What's the gestation period in my species? What's it? It's uh what's it like 5 months or something? I've been here 3 weeks and already she's given birth. Whoa, what a guy. What a And they fall for it. They fall for it every time. Females by having these pseudoestrous things, the males are not
infanticidal afterward when the female gives birth. So this whole world that initially makes no sense at all. Oh my god. animals killing members of their own species, even babies, making vicious logic when applying issues of individual selection and the qualifications by way of kin selection. Next example, another one with a similar flavor. And yeah, but wouldn't evolution favor the male that didn't figure out that it was his children? Yep. And I think probably the most technical Darwinian way of explaining that is the well-known propensity of blood flow to either go to male brains or penises.
They just get impulsive at that point. They're they don't stop to think and get the gynecology textbook that you know there no doubt is an advantage for the tiny subset of male langanger monkeys who can actually like be like prudent at that point and think through the logic. It's it works with males. they uh they fall for it. Um suggesting that a lot of what's going on there is not a conscious cognitive strategy and this is sort of not a facicious answer when by the end of next week we get to looking at how do
animals recognize individuals with a lot of species there's not a cognitive strategy you're not able to sit there and figure out wait a second this isn't making any sense so I mean like if it's not been logically figured out but wouldn't evolution favor the males that for many reasons whether they figure it out or they have to feel like that they do after three weeks instead of five months. I think we see that I think what it is is that there's not a whole lot of males there who could make sense or who have been
selected for making sense of the fact that mating has something to do with babies down at the other end of things. Um, I think there's not been much selection for being able to cognitively do that or to have some sort of rule. If I mate with this individual, anytime she produces a baby, I'm going to be unaggressive to the baby unless it's within this sort of time span. I think that's a little too much to vast to evolve in sort of a primate system. Um, the males fall for it. another version with a lot of
the same logic. And this became clear studies of baboons, Savannah baboons in the early 60s, which is you would have some male who male was having attention with another adult male and they are about to have some sort of clear fight. And one guy is obviously lowranking and he's going to get trounced. And here comes the big highranking male and this guy is terrified. And what does he do? He looks around frantically and grabs a baby and holds it to his chest. 1950s National Geographic special. Why is he doing that? Because infants are comforting. Because
babies are cute and everybody becomes less aggressive when there's a baby around cuz oh my god, who would attack somebody holding an infant? What if the infant's injured? Okay, so that's uh that's the old version. So applying some contemporary interpretations, what do you wind up seeing? Along comes the threatening male. The subordinate male grabs an infant and holds it. And what you only begin to see after a while of studying individuals is it's not random which infant he grabs. And you know exactly what's going to come next. Very similar theme to the competitive infanticide. When
males are doing kidnapping like this, and that is the term to used, they're not randomly grabbing kids. They're grabbing kids who are likely to be the offspring of that male. And it is very clear at that point, mess with me and your kids going to get it. And it's clearly not being played out in any sort of conscious level like that. But what you see is it's not random. And it comes with the sort of qualifiers that show you even more how the system is working. So you've got some guy who's a big highranking male
and has obviously fathered all sorts of kids in this troop and you're about to trounce this smaller guy who grabs one of the kids who's more likely to be his than yours. Logical competitive strategy of essentially blackmail. You've got a big high ranking male who's dominating everybody else except he only joined the troop two weeks ago. In other words, there's not been enough time for him to have kids yet. When he is threatening lower ranking guys, they're less likely to kidnap against him than kidnap against a high ranking male with a long residency. And what
you find even more interesting, which throws you into this world of, well, are these guys consciously thinking about this, or do they just smell that that kids that that kid is that kid's is that guy's kid? And do they just know in primates, they're thinking about it. And the way to realize that is when they screw up and make a mistake. And this one sort of years ago, I was watching some baboons and there was this lowranking guy. He was actually middle ranking. And this high-ranking guy who was coming at him and he was a
middle- ranking guy who critically was on his way down. He used to be a highranking guy. He was aging. In other words, he used to be reproductively active. In other words, there's some of his kids around in the troop. So, he's sitting there and here comes this terrifying highranking male who's coming at him. And our guy gets all agitated and nervous and looks around and he grabs a kid and it's likely to be his kid. He's grabbed his own kid. Oh my god. You're sitting there saying, "I'm never gonna get a damn thesis out of
this. These animals just ruined all the theoretical models." He's grabbing the wrong kid. So, he's holding the kid there and you're sitting there and you could see the guy's about five steps away and he So, he does that and tosses the kid just as he's attacked. You will see they act as if they have made a mistake. They are consciously working around something like this. So this whole notion of coercive blackmail and kidnapping again it only makes sense once you begin to see these structures of individual selection, kin selection, reciprocal altruism. Another example, and this
one makes perfect sense instantly. You look at the world of female primates and lots of different species and there's ranking systems. There's a hierarchy and what's the hierarchy built around? You get a rank one below that of your mother. You inherit your rank. Your mom is the alpha female. You're her first daughter. So, you're number two. And as soon as she has another daughter, your kid sister is number three until you have a first daughter who pushes your kid's sister down one step. In other words, dominance hierarchies amongst the females are entirely nepotistic. Whoa. How
can you explain that? Part number two, kin selection. dominant systems in all sorts of social species are built around the nepotism of relatedness. So that makes sense. Next one, next interesting notion here, which is one of those choices, one of those choices that are not conscious choices, but you're about to get pregnant and you have a choice as whatever species you are. Do you want to have a male or a female? Or do you want to have a litter of males or females? Or do you want to have a litter that's predominant male or predominantly
female? And it comes down to an issue now of two things which is how much does it cost to have a female versus a male during pregnancy and what are the reproductive probabilities of having a male versus a female. Back to this issue, you are in a big tournament species and as we saw there's high degrees of male variability in reproductive success. 5% of the guys are accounting for 95% of the of the mates. So you s you sit there and what you've got is a rule that if you go for a son like 90%
of sons running around there are never going to reproduce and you hit the jackpot and you have the 5% at the very top and each of them are going to like father 30 different kids. Going for males in a tournament species is a big gamble. It's a risky move. going for a female. However, there's no female primate out there who has 420 kids because she lays eggs like a salmon. Most females there, unless they have some fertility problem, they all have something roughly one to five kids or so over the course of the life of
an old world primate. So, female variability is way down. In other words, what's a conservative strategy to pass on copies of your genes? Have a daughter. What's a riskier strategy? have a son. And what that immediately predicts is two things. Number one, you look in dominance hierarchies and the prediction is that females who are high ranking should show more of a tendency towards having sons than daughters. And females who are low ranking exactly the opposite. And that's what you see in a bunch of primate species that have this sort of structure. Next prediction. You should
then predict that when ecological circumstances get tough. When times are tough, you want to go for the offspring that costs less. A female fetus is less calorically demanding than a male fetus. Male fetuses cost more to bring to term than females do. The prediction should be during times of ecological pressure, the percentage of females being born should increase. And as a measure of the fact that males are more expensive as fetuses, something like 53% of fertilizations in humans are males. About a 53 to 47% ratio. And over the course of pregnancy, the cost, the increased
metabolic vulnerability of male fetuses are such that by the time birth comes around, it's around 5149. And it's not until adolescence that it flips over to the typical pattern of female dominance. You've got to have more male fetuses to start off with because they are more expensive, more vulnerable. So this prediction during times of ecological duress, you should get a bias towards more females being born. The 50/50 ratio skewing in that direction. And that's precisely what you wind up seeing. And you see all sorts of examples of this in humans, for example, during periods of
famine, food deprivation, the ratio of birth skewed towards females. What you also see as a measure of that is among humans, a boy giving birth to a boy statistically decreases the body weight is likely to decrease the body weight of the next offspring. It's expensive having one of those males. And what you wind up getting then is fluctuation as a function of your dominance rank. If you're high ranking, it's almost always worth the gamble to go for one of those high-risk, high payoff boys. If you're low ranking, go for the far more conservative female. You've
got this fluctuation around this 50-50 ratio. And this was something worked out by one of these sort of founding figures, the sort of modern evolutionary thinking, a guy named Robert Trivers in the 1970s, sex ratio fluctuation as a function of social context. And people have gone and looked and it's precisely this. You get an interesting bit of conservatism in this though, which is there's some circumstance where it makes perfect sense for you to have a gazillion daughters because that's the time to do it. And at some point you're having a gazillion daughters and everybody else
is having gazillion daughters and suddenly males become really valuable because there's not a whole lot of them around. So the logical thing to do then is to switch over and start having males and everything else being equal after a while with a with a predominance of males it's going to make sense to switch over to females you have density dependent selection you will always have oscillating around 50% whichever sex is in the smaller number that one is immediately more preferable poor ecological conditions pushes you this way dominance rank push you one way or the other
but in any of those circumstances you have an awesome oscillation around the mean whichever is more common is less desirable. So sex ratio fluctuation more stuff role of kinship for example in vervet monkeys we talked about that the other day play the sound an infant alarm call and the mother gets all agitated and everyone else looks at the mother they know that she is the mother sort of demonstration of awareness of kinship there. Now another very interesting social structure to some species which is we've seen all of these tournament species. You have one high-ranking male
mating with lots of females and all that sort of stuff in polygamous systems. What about polyandry circumstances where a female is mating with multiple males? What about circumstances of stable polyandry where you get in effect the inverse of a herm? You get a single breeding female with a number of males. And what is seen with a very very high predominance is when polyandri occurs, you get a type which is called adelfic polyandry. And the second I tell what it is, it will make perfect wonderful sense. Okay. Two male lions sharing a pride. That's like not
what you're supposed to see. It's one male lion and Ma Mufasa and his like brother gets pushed out of there and like that's not what you see when you study Disney lions. There's supposed to be only one male as the breeder and the pride and and occasionally you see these prides where instead there are two males. How can they pull that off? They should be doing competitive infanticide all of that. Who are the two males? Yes, you guessed it somewhere up there before it'll be two brothers. When you see cases of two male lions sharing
a pride or flipped the other way, when you see a pride being willing to tolerate two males in there instead of one, very very high likelihood that they are brothers, this technical term adelfic polyandri. And you wind up seeing one totally wild example of this in humans. As I went over the other day, looking at our humans tournament species, they pair bonding. We're somewhere stuck in between. were terribly confused. What you see is most cultures traditionally allow polygamy, but most people are not actually polygamous. All of that. And somewhere in there, you got to ask
the same question. Hey, is there any polyandry going on with humans? And there is one wild cultural example of this. And this is seen in traditional Tibetan society. And there you get a delphic polyandry. You have the following structure in rural areas. A woman will marry a man and in the process she will marry him along with all of his brothers. All of his brothers, she marries the entire lot of them. It is adelfic polyandry. And you see that and like it's down to the point where here's this woman with her husband and his younger
brother and his younger brother all the way down to this infant that she's holding who's the youngest brother who's now one of her husbands. And that's what you see. What's the explanation for this? This is a pattern that you get in agriculturally very impoverished areas where you got a problem. You've got five sons or whatever and with a pattern of land inheritance where you would otherwise split up the land amongst the five sons that's going to put each of them below subsistence level. What you need to do is have a way in which they remain
as one reproductive unit. So you don't split up the land. You see this adelfic polyandry, a woman marrying this whole bunch of brothers and it's in circumstances where it's trying to keep the small farm plots from being broken up. But again, the logic there is the same as you see in lions with two brothers sharing a pride. What else? Now, somewhere in there, you begin to get in a realization that not only do you have males competing with males for reproductive success and females with females, but there's interexual competition. How could this be? How could
a planet that comes up with Valentine's Day have interexual competition? What is obvious is the reproductive interests of any given individual may not be exactly the same as the individual she or he mates with. What would be an example of this? Here you have a species in which males are migratory. It's a tournament species. A male shows up during a mating season. Lots of aggression, lots of secondary sexual whatever. And he mates with a female and she has kids. Hooray. They have they have parented a offspring and they've shared their genes and increased their reproductive
success. Except this being the species, this guy is going to pack up and leave the next day. In other words, he has no investment in the future reproductive health of that female, whereas she sure does. She would be delighted for this offspring to thrive and prosper, but perhaps not at the cost of her future reproduction. What we see here is between the male and the female, different reproductive strategies. And this opens up a whole bizarre world of interexual competition. Here's one really bizarre manifestation of this. Okay, basic mandelian genetics. Those of you who got the
catchup this week should have a vague sense of this by now. Hardy Weinberg ratios. You've got one parent has one genotype just to distinguish the parents and changes the colors there. All of that. What is it that isn't bothered with from day one? When you learn these sorts of things when you're make it doesn't matter which one is the male and which is the female, which is the father and which is the mother. This is just one of them contributes this profile one that what pops out the other side. basic mandelian genetics. When making sense
of pedigrees, it doesn't matter which parent is contributing the homozygotic profile, the hetereroyg, it doesn't matter which parent. But then there's a whole world of genetic traits where in fact it does matter which parent it comes from because the same combination of alals will function differently whether it came from the mother or from the father. And this was a field that sort of emerged in the early 1990s. And these are called imprinted genes. Imprinted genes. These are genes where for our purposes to define them, these are genes which have different manifestations, different phenotypic consequences. These
are genes that work differently depending on which parent they came from. Mendle rolling in his grave. This is not how it's supposed to work. This obscure world of imprinted genes violates this. They work differently depending on which parent you get it from. Dramatic foreshadowing. When we get to the ethology lecture, the word imprinting is going to come up again in a totally different sense. This is a purely molecular term right now. Okay. So, you get imprinted genes. Imprinted genes. People began to figure this out in the 90s that wait a second, we've got a completely
different profile in these individuals with a mutation in a particular disease. Here we have these two very different diseases and they turn out to have the exact same mutation in the same gene. What's up with that? If you get that gene from this parent, you get this disease. If you get it from that parent, you get that disease. Totally boggling. Making no sense at all. first thing to have to be solved. What's a mechanism for actually making a gene work differently depending on which parent it comes from? For those who care about these sorts of
details, it's a biochemical process called methylation. You methylate the gene in one parent's genome and not the other. Don't worry about the details. The main way is there is a genetic mechanism for making genes work differently depending on which parent it comes from. So, okay, we know how to do it. Why should this happen? Why does this make any sense? And it took a while and people began to see patterns to this. And finally, this was put together by an evolutionary biologist at Harvard named David Hey in the early 90s who came up with a
unifying theory which explains it all. And it's very cool. Here's what you see. You begin to look at imprinted genes. And for our purposes, the way we can describe them as this is a gene where if you get it from one parent, it does what it's supposed to do. If you get it from another parent, it's silenced. It never works. It's methylated into silence. These are imprinted genes. So he began to notice something. When you look at imprinted genes where they are active, if they come from the father, they all tend to be genes that
promote fetal growth. They're all genes that increase fetal metabolism. They're all genes that make for a thicker uterus. They're all genes driven by hormones coming out of the fetus. They're all genes that push for greater fetal development. And then you look at the imprinted genes coming from the female and they're all genes that tend to slow down fetal development. What have we got here? We've got that interexual competition played out precisely there. You've got this male hamster who's going to mate and never be seen again because he's in this category. And what does he want?
He could care less what happens to the future reproductive success of this female. He wants this offspring of his to survive. The male imprinted genes push for greater amounts of fetal growth. and the female ones saying, "Well, that would be great if this kid survives, but I also have a future reproductive life to think about." Her imprinted genes tend to counter it. What would this look like? One of the imprinted genes, first one's identified, which comes from active from the male codes for a protein called insulin like growth factor. Who cares what insulin like growth
factor does? What's clear from the name is it's a growth factor. It's one of those things that make fetuses grow more. It promotes fetal growth. And then meanwhile, the female hamster has a cognate, an opposing imprinted gene. What's that one for? That codes the gene for the insulin-like growth factor receptor. And what her version does is make for a less responsive receptor. The male is pushing for more of the insulin-like growth factor. She gets a receptor that's not as responsive. And you literally have this co-evolutionary arms race there of male imprinted genes evolving to push
for more and more fetal growth, female ones pushing for less and less fetal growth. And you see that coming out most apparently when you see mutations in some of these genes. For example, another pair of imprinted genes, one from the father, one from the mother. And what you see there is the mother's version, the father's version promotes invasion, placental invasion into the uterus. And that's actually the term that gynecologists use, invasion of the fetus into the uterine wall. It promotes more of that. The female version slows it down. So what if you have a disease
where the male gene relevant to this is mutated and knocked out of action? you solely have the female input which is decreasing fetal implantation into the uterus. What have you got then? You've got a disease where fertilized eggs don't implant. On the other side, suppose there's a mutation in the female part of this pairing and as a result all you have is the male input driving for more and more aggressive fetal invasion. What do you have? you wind up with one of the all-time bad cancers. You don't want to get choreocarcinoma, a cancer of the
uterus because it's growing completely out of control being prompted by the fetus. When you take out each of the voices through mutation, you see that normally you're having this tilting, this balancing of competition between males pushing for more fetal growth at the expense of the future reproductive success of the female, females trying to slow it down. All of the imprinted genes show this. Amazingly, people now have even identified imprinted genes that don't work until after birth and they work in the brain. And what do they do? The malederived one does things like make for infants
that suckle more. Ooh, get more calories out of mom. And the fetal version tends to blunt some of the suckling reflexes. is all built around the strategy of males and females do not necessarily have exactly overlapping reproductive strategies. So another version of it is one a male gene which is pushing towards expression of a fetal enzyme fetal lactogen placental lactogen. And what that does is it makes it easier for the fetus to grab sugar out of the bloodstream from the mother and the mother tries to counter it. And if she's not very effective at doing
it or if she's rather overeffective, you get pregnancy hypoglycemia, pregnancy diabetes. Suddenly, this is mom having a fight with her offspring over how much calories they're going to get. The offspring being driven by an imprinted gene from the father. Totally cool. Totally interesting. There should be a problem lurking here, though, which is this is a pattern you only see in tournament species. You got a pair bonding species. And there's no reason why a male should be saying something like, "Oo, I want to like ruin her future reproductive success at the cost of her giving birth.
I'm a I'm a pair bonding bowl, and I wanted to give birth to a child the size of an elephant who's going to survive. And who cares about her future?" They're pair bonding. They're in it together for the rest of time. You don't find imprinted genes in pair bonding species. You find imprinted genes in tournament species. And thus we get back to that same issue the other day looking at tournament versus par where do humans fall into this and what we've already seen is if humans can come up with choreocarcinomomas and things like that we
have imprinted genes same punchline again as the other day in terms of the number of genes we have puts us somewhere in between tournament and pair bonding species again we are terribly confused. Okay, let's take a 5m minute break and we will pick up with more examples. Okay, let's get going again. Uh, two good questions just now. One is where does homosexuality fit into all of this? And where it fits in is about 30 minutes worth of the sexual behavior lecture sometime in midmay. It is a challenge for some of this thinking. Uh, the second
good question was, "Am I capable of speaking louder?" I will try. I mumble. Okay, pushing on. So, we've just brought in this whole bizarre, unexpected world of trashing the Hallmark cards of interexual com competition. Another example of it, a really interesting one. This was work done by a guy named William Rice at Santa Cruz over some years looking at a number of different fly species. Female flies are polyanderous. They mate with a lot of different males and you're a male who've just mated with a female. And what you would like more than anything in order
to pass on copies of your own genes is like not have some other guy impregnate her, some other male fly guy who's intimidating you, not have some other guy do it. And suddenly what you see is this interesting world where because of the mating frequency of the females, she will have sperm from a number of different males inside her at the same time. And suddenly we get this very strange world, a whole field of research of sperm competition, sperm competition of relay teams and gold medals and all of that of sperm competing with the sperm
from other males. And what you see is in fly species, the sperm of males make toxins that kill the sperm of other males. Whoa. That's very elegant. That's very elegant because it in part requires you to come up with a toxin that isn't toxic to yourself. You can solve that. All sorts of molecular tricks for doing that. But this makes wonderful sense. You increase the likelihood of you, the sperm, reaching the goal line and killing the other guys and all of that. Except there is a problem for the females, which is these toxins that the
male sperm release aren't such a hot deal for the female's health. And what Rice did were these really interesting studies. I won't go into the details of it in large part because I still don't understand what the guy did. But he was able to somehow take populations of male and female flies and he would hold the females so that they could not evolve in response to whatever was going on with the males, malemale competition for reproduction. While he held constant the female female competition, the females were not evolving, the males were. And what he saw
over the course of 30 generations was this male male sperm competition was such that the male sperm were making such powerful toxins that they were shortening the life expecties of the females. Whoa, that's not smart. Whoa, that makes perfect sense. House flies are not your parabonding swans dying in each other's arms for life sort of organisms. It's a classic case of the male only has an investment in the current reproductive bout and if he can wipe out the competitor's sperm and oh bummer the female has now got all sorts of rotty necrotic lesions in her
vagina. This is not a very reproductively effective female fly down the line. What do I care? I'm off to the next pile of cow dung after that. You have that same structure there. But this is a bummer for the females. The males left to their own competition will evolve more and more toxic sperm which exacts more and more of a price on the future reproductive success of the females. Now Rice flips things and now he takes these populations and he holds the male evolution constant and allows the females to evolve and over the force course
of 30 generations they have evolved means of detoxifying the sperm. So here we have malemale competition inadvertently being played out in an interexual realm as well where again this co-evolutionary arms race totally bizarre sperm killing each other and sperm damaging the female and that makes no sense if you you know do for the good of the species again very logical in the context of these models these models of evolution. Okay, final example, and here's one that will become very relevant way down the line when we're looking at things like the biology of aggression. So, you've
got these social primates, and in lots of social mammals, you've got this pattern, which is one of the genders picks up around puberty and moves to another group. It's to avoid inbreeding and everybody having six fingers and tails if you're a species that doesn't have a tail. And it's perfectly logical. And what you need is just some sort of pattern. Is it the females or the males of the species which disperse? And no one among the primates has ever found a good rule for which primate species have female exogamy or male exogamy. Which gender leaves
at puberty, but there's variability. In chimps, it's the females who leave at puberty. In gorillas, it's the females who leave at puberty. in baboons, macaks, a few other old world monkeys. It's the males who leave at puberty with a very critical implication. So you've got baboons. Baboons, you've got a troop and you look at the adult males and adult females in there. The adult females grew up in that group. The adult males grew up someplace else and immigrated into here at puberty. In other words, among the adult females in this group, they're all relatives. Among
the males, there's no relatives. Meanwhile, next door with the chimps, it's the females who have left at puberty. You look at the mature animals in a group, and it's the males who are there with all the relatives who have been there all their life and the females who have no relatives. So, asking a question, comparing baboons and chimps, which of those species has higher levels of male male aggression within the group? Baboons. Yeah. because they're all brothers and brothers at arms and the chimp group there and none of the adult males and baboon troops are
typically related to each other. Very high levels of intermale aggression introup. So which species on a certain level has put us to shame when it comes to the invention of warfare of fighting between groups? Chimps. Because you've got bunches of males who cooperate because they are relatives. And what you wind up seeing with chimps, and we will eventually get to this, is you see things that are now termed border patrols, where a bunch of males from a group will patrol the edge of their territory. If they encounter a male from the group over there, they
will attack. They will kill him. As documented by Jane Goodall, taken to an extreme, the males of one group will eradicate another group. And if the rule is I am killing this guy, we are killing this guy. Not because we don't like the look on his face, but because he is a member of that group. This is the United Nations definition of genocide. Chimps have not only come up with organized warfare between groups, they have come up with a chimp version of genocide. What's the driving force on that? One of the truly scary things on
this planet, which is when all the males living next door are getting along with each other. Because when they do, they suddenly start looking over at this side. organized males driven by female exogamy and chimps is where the warfare pattern comes from. And as you can guess, when we get to thinking about the biology of aggression with humans, we are suddenly in this world of taking non relatives who are military recruits and convincing them to pseudo kinship themselves into being a band of brothers, increasing the sense of kinship there. Chimps do this all on their
own. So now making sense of which primate species have high levels of male male aggression within group versus between group between group are ones where it's the females who leave at puberty. All these guys have been together since they were terrorizing other kids in kindergarten. They're an organized group against each other. You get intergroup organized conflict in primates that have female exogamy. Females leaving a puberty. Okay. So we've now worked our way through a bunch of examples here and how to apply individual selection, kin selection, that sort of thing. A fourth branch has come into
the field in the last decade or so. And this is one that has had a lot of controversy in part because what is built around is mistaken by a lot of folks as being an outdated concept, but in part because what it is actually implying is very controversial in some realms. And this is this business of group selection somehow sneaking back in the back door there and becoming relevant to the evolution of social behavior. Group selection, you remember Wednesday, all the wilderbeast there and the elderly wilderbeast pushes his way forward and distributes his bank account
and then leaps into the river and the crocs and all of that. And that makes no sense at all. And because animals don't behave for the good of the species and individual selection, he got pushed in. Nonetheless, a notion of group selection has come back into thinking, but it occurs only under a couple of specialized circumstances. The version that we just trashed, which was the version Marlon per Okay, how many of you guys know who Marlon Perkins was? It's come to that Marlon Perkins. How many of you guys know what does Mutual of Omaha, does
that still exist? Mutual of Omaha's Wild Kingdom, is that still a television program? Yes. No. when you were young. Did that exist? Okay. Is it still sponsored by Mutual of Omaha? They still do. That's great. Okay. Because they've been that one has been going for decades and decades. And you know, it was always, you know, they'd have they they'd somehow always have to segue the Mutual of Omaha in there during the commercials building and it like, you know, just as rhinos will mate on for hours on end, you want fire insurance for your home or
some such thing. But in its original couple of decades, it was hosted by a guy named Marlon Perkins. And Marlon Perkins taught most of America their evolutionary biology. Marlon Perkins is the guy who taught everybody behaving for the good of the species and that sort of thing. The first version of group selection that got trashed in the 60s was Marlon Perkins group selection. The group selection that's sneaking back into the field is very different. Two different versions. First one, something you will get in various populations. You've got a population of something or others and some
biogeographic event occurs which causes a subset of them to get isolated from everybody else. A landbridge disappears or somebody drops a lake there or who knows what, but something isolates a small subset of the population. So these guys go about reproducing by their standards and these guys go about reproducing by their standards. And what's going to be a characteristic very soon of the smaller population? They're going to be more inbred than this population simply because they're smaller. They're going to have a higher degree of relatedness among individuals. So now we throw our second piece of
our three building blocks. Throw in kin selection. And what that immediately predicts is levels of cooperation will be higher in this group than in this group because the higher degree of relatedness. That's great. And that's great because once we see how sort of cooperative systems have a larger payoff, you will get a crystallization so that everybody the second or third cousins are eventually going to have to be just as cooperative as the siblings are with each other. you are going to fix a trait of cooperation in that population at a high rate. So notice here
in this case we have a high degree of cooperation driven by kin selection whereas these guys are going about their usual savage at each other's throats business. biogeographic event reverses and these guys get reinccorporated into the main population. And they're so different by then that they get a different color. And what you've got then is here in the large population, here is a nucleus of animals who are cooperating. And here is the huge unwashed mass of the ones who are not. And everything we know about reciprocal altruism, all of that means that these guys are
going to start out competing these guys. And the model that is used is cooperation will have to crystallize outward. Animals will have to join in in these cooperative patterns because these guys will out compete them. What do we have? This is called a founder effect. This is a small population that thanks to being inbred has fixated some trait that's advantageous where the evolution here moves faster than in here because of the smaller population with the inbreeding. Some adaptive trait comes in there. They get reinduced into the main population and this founderdriven trait quickly spreads throughout
the population. And what you get here is if the same thing is occurring in the realm of behavior, reciprocal cooperative behavior, somewhere in here, you have a transition from this being a kin selection phenomenon to this being a reciprocal altruism phenomenon. So that stands as one of the models out there for how do you jumpstart cooperation in non relative populations? You use inbred founder populations to drive it there. And just to have a metaphor, all you need to do think about in some city there's some occupation, some merkantile, something or other that there's a gazillion
of them and all sorts of people work in that and there's a subgroup of people there who work in this who are all related and as a result they do something cooperative and kin selective. They make lowinterest loans to each other and as a result they're more successful at business and they get incorporated into here and what you then have is this economic force that everybody else has to join in to this cooperative business of trusting each other or lowinterest loans or I trust you like a sibling you don't have to pay me next week
used to be that only siblings trusted each other like siblings but a force there for more cooperation a driving force course of inbred kin selection initially. So that's one way in which people are thinking about group selection and here you now have this group out competing this because of this trait and people actually use terms like crystallization the trait of cooperation will crystallize outward and fix in the whole population. So that's one version of it and this is one again we will come back to this is one of the ways to jumpstart cooperation in a
world in which there is no reciprocal altruism because you see the problem with that we all have learned tit for tat is the best one except forgiving tit for tat might be better except pavlo might even be better and all of those are built around this one requirement though which is somebody has to make the first move of being cooperative that's got to jumpstart the system. And what we all know is in a system that doesn't go along with the rules of can't we all sing kumbaya, what happens is the first one in the first
round who does something cooperative is some schmuck who's one step behind everybody else for the rest of time. There's no way to get these cooperative systems to evolve initially. We'll see there are ways and this is one of those have it driven initially by kin selection and then throw it into the general population. So that's one version of where group selection is crept back in. Another version is sort of the more generalized form of it. And it could be illustrated, okay, the following example. You've got chickens. You've got chickens with an array of traits. And
you can see there's two types of chickens. One chicken is super aggressive and beats on everybody else and she lays lots of eggs. another chicken far more pacific and introspective and less aggressive and has fewer eggs. So now you take any one of the high aggression females and one of the low aggression guys and put them together and who's going to leave more copies of her genes? The high aggression female laying lots of eggs. Now instead you have a whole group of the high aggression females and a whole group of the low aggression females. What's
going to happen in the high aggression females? They're all so aggressive that they all injure each other. They all stress each other into lower fertility rates. All of that. And suddenly you have this very important world in which A can dominate B, but where groups of B dominate groups of A. And it's in that realm that you suddenly get a push towards group selection. When selection on that level, a world of any individual being outco competed because of a trait, but as a collective and bringing in that word, not randomly, as a collective and the
collectivist future of chicken, low-ranking chickens to throw off their chains. Once you have a group of them, the same traits that could be disadvantageous on an individual diatic level as a group out competes the others. And when you do that, you've suddenly got group selection in a classical form where these animals because of their traits, they are not behaving for the good of the group, but the very traits that are disadvantageous individually are advantageous as a group. And it's another way of thinking about this. If you indiscriminately make lowinterest loans to everybody on Earth, you
are very readily falling into this category. If you are part of a group that makes lowinterest loans to each other, suddenly you out compete the other. Fascinating book. Evolutionary biologist named David Sloan Wilson, who's been the main person pushing for this idea for decades, has a very broad range of intellectual interests, including religious history. And a number of years ago, he published a book called Darwin's Cathedral. And he analyzed the birth of clusters of new religions over time as examples of founder effects and group selection type properties. And he wound up in great detail analyzing
Calvin and the starts of Calvinism and Calvin and his sidekick Hobbes and Zurich in the 17th century or whenever it was that Calvinism started of analyzing how there were all sorts of these little religions popping up there in these little city states and what it was about Calvinism in I think it was Zurich that took off where they established some of these inbred cooperative patterns and then beat the pants off of all the other city states and soon all of central Europe was Calvinist. Really interesting book in that regard. Okay, so back has come this
whole possibility of group selection and out of that has come the more broad way of thinking about it now which is multilevel selection. A very important concept here which is sometimes selection occurs entirely on the level of a single gene. In other words, have a gene with bad enough of mutation and it doesn't matter how many great other genes you got going for you. Selection will be decided entirely on the basis of one gene. As we saw in our principles from the other day, much more common selection at the level of the individual, an aggregation
of the genotype playing out in terms of phenotype, all the different traits there. But what this introduces is some circumstances where selection is at the group level. And it is never at the level of how an individual's trait plays out individually in a group. It's how this trait played out individually emerges as a group behavior that you get group selection. And this marks this great piece that's come between this David Sloan Wilson and the guy who's probably the most towering figure in this whole field. this guy Eio Wilson, Edward O. Wilson at Harvard who David
Sloan Wilson and Eio Wilson spent years not getting along with each other because Wilson Wilson because Eio Wilson is very strongly individual selectionist model guy and thus he hated group selection. David Sloan Wilson the other way around. Each of them were endlessly invited to the wrong parties because of the same same shared last name and all sorts of social awkwardness there in the last few years. I don't know what happened. I don't know if they arranged marriages between their grandchildren or something, but they arranged some sort of truce. And Wilson Wilson published a paper last
year basically saying, "Whoa, you know what? Some of the time I'm right and some of the time he's right, and isn't that great? And can't we all get along?" And delineating circumstances where individual selection, individual selection, kin selection, reciprocal altruism is going to dominate circumstances where selection is going to get played out at the group level. They love each other. Now they have formed a new language. They share burial customs and this sort of constitutes a great resolution. Yeah. Depending on the circumstances, the most important thing in evolution might be a single gene, a single
organism or a single group. And the power of a group and aggregate traits that work lousy in the individual is going to be really important down the line when thinking about aggression and stopping aggression and things of that sort. Okay. So all of this is great. This multi-level selection now showing sort of a fancier more sophisticated way of thinking about individual kin reciprocal circumstances where it works circumstances where this is the most interesting thing. So that's great. So now we begin to look for a first pass at where humans fit into this. We've already seen
in terms of all of these traits, tournament versus pair bonding, we're somewhere in between. In terms of imprinted genes, we're somewhere in between. These are specifics. How do we begin for a first pass to think about humans, human behavior fitting into these frameworks? Two problems or two issues that are all we'll touch on at this point. The first one is there's this temptation as soon as you think along these lines to discover, oh, isn't that interesting? Langanger monkeys will have infanticide. Male langanger monkeys will kill the kids of some other male. Human males do that
sometimes. Oh, langanger monkeys might be an interesting evolutionary model for where infanticidal behavior came from in humans. Oh, isn't this interesting? Dominance is passed along by males. We do that some. Isn't it interesting? Kidnapping somehow occurs and before you know it, you have a world in which people are saying, "Well, damsel flies do something that look like rape." So, huh, we've got a damsel model of human behavior in that realm and two things are happening there. One is taking the traits, a small number of traits in some other species and inflating the relevance it has
for understanding us. The other danger is somewhere creeping below the surfaces. You see, it happens in other species. It's kind of natural. It's kind of inevitable. This is a whole world in which people have run off with how this applies to humans in a completely distorted way. We are unique as a species as is every other species. We are unique by applying the same evolutionary rules that every other species has. That's the commonality, not the outcome. The other challenge is constantly going to be that you get all these great predictive models and then humans go
and screw it up by having quirks and idiosyncrasies and personalities and stuff like that. All I can say in that realm is back to Wednesday. If making sense of which lions are going to go to the scary speaker in the bushes there revolves in some part around personalities, humans by the time you get to us blindly assuming these models apply is going to be very sort of unshaky very shaky. Okay. So just a first pass at how one will have to be cautious for the rest of the course and applying all these models. Now far
more critically is looking at what are the criticisms that have been brought against this whole framework. This framework of social behavior has evolved to maximize number of copies of genes driven by kin driven by reciprocal altruism. Group selection is a rare trait but when it does it follows these properties. This is how you explain all of behavior. know these four or five rules and not only can you look at two skulls of different sizes but you can explain all of behavior and all of human behavior and it's all biology and eventually as Eio Wilson said
in this landmark book of his in 1975 sociobiology eventually all of the social sciences will be under the wing of evolutionary biology no surprise the social scientists were not very pleased with that proclamation at that point but this was this notion that this was going to explain everything. So, what are the criticisms that have been leveled at this style of thinking? And as we'll see, there are a lot of them. All of the stuff we've been thinking about, this basic notion of applying the Darwinian rules as to why giraffe have to have hearts of certain
size to behavior, there's three features that are coming throughout. The first one is the notion of heritability of traits because what we've been discussing throughout the last lecture or so is there's heritability of the tendency to kidnap infants at the right strategic time. There's heritability of the tendency to decide that you like males with big fancy plumage and long canines as opposed. There's heritability to deciding that your daughter should be highest ranking and the true brother of that one's heritable heritability. An assumption of heritable behavioral traits runs through every single one of these discussions. The
next theme that runs throughout all of them is the notion of adaptiveness. If some trait has emerged out of the evolutionary merc, if some trait has been selected for, selection implies adaptiveness. the notion that everything you see has an adaptive explanation for why it's emerged evolutionarily. The third principle which is not explicitly stated in this field very often but is implicit to everything about it is these processes of evolutionary change are gradual at every generation. If by getting into some better behavioral strategy for playing prisoners dilemma, you leave 1% more copies of your genes than
everybody else. come back in a 100red zillion generations and that trait will have spread. All of this is played out generation after generation so that evolutionary change is occurring in gradual incremental steps. So what do you do with these three starting points? The first one, an awful lot of the next two weeks we'll be looking at how other disciplines look at the issue of how do you figure out when a behavior is genetic? And one of the first things we'll learn is how that phrase means absolutely nothing whatsoever. Maybe a better way of stating it
is how do you figure out when a behavioral trait has a genetic influence? And as we'll see even the safer way of saying it is how do you figure out in which environment a certain behavior has a particular genetic influence. How do other disciplines go about saying oh it's genetic. It's genetic. In this field among this sort of sociobiological thinking what you do is an inverse style of proof. You say okay if a trait has a genetic component you would expect it to be heritable along these lines. If it's more adaptive it will increase its
frequency. All of that and look that explains where competitive infanticide has come from. Show me a more explanatory model. Show me a model that is more predictive. And until you can show me one, I'm assuming this trait has some heritability and we'll see how by next week the more molecular people rip this view to shreds. Tell me when you're talking about heritability of some behavior. Show me the damn gene. Show me the sequence of DNA. Show me how that produces the behavior. This is where this field ends saying, "Oh, we can inferentially sort of decide
there's a genetic influence." As we'll see by next week, this one piece is the main thing attacked by a number of other fields. Next piece, the adaptiveness. the notion that everything that has evolved has evolved under the scalpel of evolutionary demand for optimization and all of that. And the critics of this view call this the adaptationist fallacy. The notion that everything is there for a good reason. And what they often do in terms of making fun of that is that oh you look at people who do this kind of evolutionary biology with this adaptationist emphasis
and it's one big just so story. Why do giraffe have hearts with thick muscles? And here's the story I make up. And if my story is better than yours, I get tenure and you don't. Why is it the zebras got stripes? Why is it that males kill infants in the species or whatever? That everything is adaptive. And what you need to do now is come up with a story to explain why it's adaptive. And if it fits with those principles of individual and group, etc., then you win. You've got the best gesso story. And what
is constantly brought as a sort of criticism of that is show me experimental evidence that it is adaptive. And by the time we get to looking at the field of ethology, you'll see a whole way in which people do that. Show me that it's possible to disprove it. Okay, tell me a just so story where if that were the case that would prove that this is not what's going on. You got to have the rules of science. It can't just be you observe something and come up with the best story and you win. But what's
most striking in sort of terms of criticizing this is some domains in which there are traits that have not evolved because they are adaptive. They've just evolved because they are excess baggage. And this has brought in a whole sort of criticism of the field. Now some of the time when you begin to see it's kind of hard to see where the adaptiveness is, it's because it's a more subtle feature of it. For example, you could look at squid and someone who is enamored with fish and how cool and aerodynamic aquadnamic fish are and all of
that will point out that squid are pretty lousy swimmers compared to all sorts of fish. They're pretty lousy swimmers compared to fish, but they're pretty great swimmers for something starting out meta evolutionarily as a mollisk. This adaptimus has to be in the context of where it came from, where it got to. Okay, so that is a qualifier. Nonetheless, even factoring that in of hidden features of adaptation, evolutionary history, there's lots of traits that are simply not adaptive. And one of the most effective critics of this whole view, Steven J. Gould, introduced a term about 30
years ago that summarized this view. Gould was a paleontologist at Harvard and his thinking will come in a whole lot in next week's lectures. He teamed up with another guy, a molecular geneticist at Harvard named Richard Leuent. And in critiquing a lot of this, they came up with a term saying an awful lot of traits have evolved that exist not because they're adaptive, but because they're spandrels. So at that point, everybody in the field had to go run to their dictionary because Gould and Lewon were all being snotty and coming up with fancy high flulutin
culture terms because it turns out spandrel is a term from architecture. So immediately they already won points off of everyone else for being all cultured and stuff. But a spandrel, a spandrel is you got some medieval building. Remarkably enough that's a medieval building and um it's a nice medieval building. And what you get is you have these arches that there winds up being a space between arches that there's space between arches just because you can't build arches next to each other without there being like the space here. This is solid brick or Lego blocks or
whatever. So you have these arches and there's a space in between and you got this space. It's not there for any good reason. It's there because you can't put two arches next to each other without coming up with this triangular space. And the architectural term for those triangles are spandrels. And spandrels in architecture is something that forms between two arches. And as Gulen Lewon used the term, a spandrel is some trait that occurs simply as the unavoidable outcome of some other traits that are being selected for. You can't put two arches together without getting some
sort of triangular shape in between. And throughout history of architects making spandrels, these would be artistically built up and all of that and there would be all sorts of decorative stuff on that. So if you are an adaptationist folly person where everything has an adaptive purpose, you look at that and oh, there's got to be some reason why spandrels have evolved and have these elaborate secondary sexual features on spandrels of little little gossamer things flying around and stuff. And there's got to be an adapt. Oh, I know. Because people in church will look up at
it a whole lot and their necks will get sore. And when their necks stop being sore, they will decide this is the glory of God. And they will increase their belief for some. That's why architects put in spandrels. No, you can't build buildings like this without spandrels happening. And just because they were there, why not? Let's decorate them. That's the evolution of spandrels. And in Genon Lewenton's critique, an awful lot of stuff where the sociobiology types would sit there and say, "We're going to have a just so story contest now and use our principles and
figure out some way in which this is actually adaptive." They're saying this actually wasn't selected for because it's adaptive. It was an inadvertent byproduct of something else. So, here's an example that would be given. Humans have chins. Humans have chins. Apparently, all humans do have chins hidden away someplace or other. Humans have chins. And weirdly, we're like the only primate that does. You look at like other apes and stuff and they have the sort of weak chins that suggest sort of immoral characters and criminology, criminality and things like that. We're the only species that has
chins. And apparently there's been some like nutty adaptationist school somewhere back when trying to make sense of why it is that humans evolved chins. why it is that human faces come to a point there and what's the adaptive advantage and you can like stab rivals or you can get crumbs from out of the corner of on the floor and stuff with your chin until somebody figured out that there's no way that you can have a primate face that has a muzzle forcehortened and a jaw at this angle. You do this and you do this and
you're going to get this little spandrel thing sticking out there and oh, there hasn't been selection for a chin because you have selection for a homoded face with a shortened muzzle. This thing pops out there and suddenly all those doctrinal thesis about the reproductive advantages of chins go down the tubes there. It was merely a spandrel. And Golden Luenton's argument is that there's lots and lots of spandrels that this emphasis everything is adaptive. Everything is the outcome of competition that's produced that an awful lot of stuff is excess baggage that merely gets carried along because
the evolutionary process isn't all that efficient. You're starting with a mollisk if you want to make yourself a squid. Giving rise to another concept, another term in the field. Andre Laf who was a French uh or was it Jacob? There were these two French molecular biology guys and they both got Nobel prizes and one of them said this and I don't remember which but I'm going to say it was Laf who did because I like saying a Laf. Andre Lewaf once said that evolution is not an inventor. Evolution is a tinkerer. It works with pre-existing
structures. And the notion that you're going to see this everywhere. You're gluing stuff together. You're duct taping this part of the pelvis to this part of the circulatory system, whatever, and you're going to have spandrels. An awful lot of what those folks go through in these whole song and dances of adaptation are merely spandrels. Evolution is not about optimizing every single trait. What comes out of that is this notion that uh an awful lot of this world of competition really isn't about competition. You stick with these models, the models we've been covering for the last
two days and every bit of advantage is going to increase the number of copies of your genes. What's the only possible outcome? You need to compete. You need to out compete. It was a critique of this whole view as being one driven very strongly by competition because all the traits there are driven by having to outrouce the next individual next individual. Another realm in which this has been critiqued has been by interestingly evolutionary biologists from the Soviet Union. And where what was emphasized there in western sort of emphasis in evolutionary biology, most of evolutionary selection
was about competing with other individuals, sexual competition, competing for running away from the predator faster than the guy next to you, competing for food, all of that. If you're sitting there in the Soviet Union, you're thinking about evolutionary biology. What you're mostly thinking about is evolving the means of surviving climate demands, surviving extremes of climate. What would be categorized as abiotic rather than biotic abiotic demands in the environment. And when you look at species where most of their survival is about surviving ecological extremes, environmental extremes, you don't see a whole lot of competition. That's a
world with far less. And throughout the years, the Soviet evolutionary biologists tended to emphasize abiotic selection, which had far less of an emphasis on competition. The final piece though, the gradualism, the gradualism is very intrinsic to this because if everything is adaptive, every little bit of advantage is worth fighting for and competition and killing each other's infants and killing each other's sperms and ovducts and all of that. And every little bit is going to make for a little bit of an advantage and you will have this gradualist change. And the huge challenge that came to
that was from Gould and another scientist in the 80s a notion that maybe that's not what evolution looks like. Maybe evolution is not occurring in these gradualist incremental steps. Maybe what occurs instead is long periods of nothing happening and then a rapid evolutionary change and long periods nothing happening. a rapid evolutionary change, something that they called punctuated equilibrium. And the whole next lecture is going to be about this critical implications. The genetics of how stuff evolves turns out to fit far better the molecular genetics with these models of punctuated rapid change and stasis, equilibrium, punctuated
change, rather than these gradualist models. And if that's what it looks like in these steps, most of the time you're not punching it out for every one half of 1% reproductive advantage and it's all competition. Most of the time nothing's happening at all and there's periods of severe selection and then long periods in which nothing happens. A step function like that, the role of competition is reduced tremendously in thinking about evolution. So, we're going to spend a ton of time looking at the punctuated equilibrium model to give away the punchline that I know you are
all on the edge of your seats for. I kind of think it's right in a lot of the circumstances of evolution because it fits much better with the molecular biology. Finally, in addition to these critiques of show me the genes, everything doesn't have to be adaptive because there are spandrels and besides this is really wimpy science where science counts as making up the best story and there's no reason to think that it's gradual. There's mechanism for this as well. In addition to that, there's always been a final realm of criticism of this style of thinking
which has been a political critique. And in lots of realms of science, you can't really imagine a political critique as to whether psyia on amoeba flip this way or that way or a critique as to whether like plants should have three clover things or four clover things that there's not a political factor in that. There's a huge amount of socopolitical implications that run through this entire field because it addresses issues of like how natural, how evolutionarily ancient, how evolutionarily honed, how adaptive are things like whether a species, a society has male domination or not, has
strict hierarchies or not, has certain degrees of aggression, has certain patterns of sexual coercion, and a whole world arguing that these things have a naturalistic grounding and adaptation versus a whole world that says that's gibberish. Most of the time nothing's happening. And when it does, a lot of it is spandrels. Those have very different political implications. And from day one, the critics of this entire world have made a point of something that initially seems kind of silly, but may not be silly, which is the entire founding generation of these sociobiologists were white southern males. Make
of that what you will. Io Wilson from Alabama, Robert Trivers, Irvin D'vor, a whole population of these folks, the first generation of these people were all white males from the American South, whereas Gould and Lewenton and all of these guys were northeastern Marxists. Okay, so we got kind of a major contrast here. And the critique of the socio-biological view is, isn't it interesting that their notion of how evolutionary evolution works just happens to emphasize the naturalness of a system that rewards them for the inequalities that we have. Isn't it interesting that their models predict the
naturalness of a world in which they're the ones who benefit most from the notion that this is natural? And from day one, that has been a strong critique. And that's not like critiquing psyia amoeba theories for political implications. That is a very real one because the notions that this is pertinent to making sense of is rape a human psychopathology or is rape a competitive strategy? Is the fact that children are more likely to be killed by stepfathers than by biological fathers? Does that represent something about the typical socioeconomic pressures in families with stepparents versus the
natural biology of gene competition through competitive infanticide? This has a lot of implications and from day one as this field emerged in the late '7s. There was a huge political agenda that was assumed to be there sufficiently so at one lecture famous conference in 1977 where Eio Wilson was presenting there a bunch of people rushed the stage knocked him off the stage and dumped water on him and chanted whatever it is in German saying we will have a society of law and order. This is pretty agitated circumstances for thinking about science. This was a group
in Boston called science for the people which is a Marxist group at the time there and saying all this sociological stuff is doing is justifying a world in which it is male-dominated stratified and where aggression and competition pays off and this has been highly controversial from the beginning. The one counter to it and I think a lot of that was valid and I think a lot of it instead defaults into models where this is not making a whole lot of sense. The one thing to be pointed out is when we see what punctuated equilibrium is
about, it's exactly the sort of world of evolution that produces the sort of world that a Gould or a Luenton would want it to be. One which minimizes competition in favor of cooperation and a whole pattern of stasis and rapid dialectical change. It's exactly the world that two Marxist geneticists would say evolution should be about. So I think as a first pass here seeing this stuff is dripping in socopolitical implications. So starting Monday we will look at this punctuated equilibrium taking apart this piece of the story and basically for the rest of the course examining
for more please visit us at stanford.edu.
Related Videos
4. Molecular Genetics I
1:33:35
4. Molecular Genetics I
Stanford
2,315,409 views
2. Behavioral Evolution
1:36:57
2. Behavioral Evolution
Stanford
7,265,659 views
Do We Have Free Will? with Robert Sapolsky & Neil deGrasse Tyson
54:48
Do We Have Free Will? with Robert Sapolsky...
StarTalk
2,038,189 views
Neuroscientist: How To Escape The Rat Race | Robert Sapolsky
1:22:30
Neuroscientist: How To Escape The Rat Race...
Light Watkins
758,945 views
4 Hours Chopin for Studying, Concentration & Relaxation
4:00:37
4 Hours Chopin for Studying, Concentration...
HALIDONMUSIC
18,896,666 views
Your Brain: Who's in Control? | Full Documentary | NOVA | PBS
53:33
Your Brain: Who's in Control? | Full Docum...
NOVA PBS Official
6,599,661 views
Coldplay Greatest Hits - Best Songs Of Coldplay - Top Songs 2025 Playlist #coldplay
1:28:39
Coldplay Greatest Hits - Best Songs Of Col...
VM Channel
64,931 views
Why Is There Only One Species of Human? - Robin May
59:22
Why Is There Only One Species of Human? - ...
Gresham College
1,462,306 views
Veritasium: What Everyone Gets Wrong About AI and Learning – Derek Muller Explains
1:15:11
Veritasium: What Everyone Gets Wrong About...
Perimeter Institute for Theoretical Physics
3,769,300 views
1. Introduction to Human Behavioral Biology
57:15
1. Introduction to Human Behavioral Biology
Stanford
18,535,317 views
Science of Stress, Testosterone & Free Will | Dr. Robert Sapolsky
1:29:50
Science of Stress, Testosterone & Free Wil...
Andrew Huberman
1,626,615 views
The Complete Chopin Nocturnes 432 Hz
1:57:50
The Complete Chopin Nocturnes 432 Hz
Classical Oasis
1,126,356 views
World War II: Part 1 – WWI
1:57:23
World War II: Part 1 – WWI
Dr. Roy Casagranda
1,408,478 views
Why the rush? - lo-fi beats for work/study / cat jazz
3:31:19
Why the rush? - lo-fi beats for work/study...
chill chill journal
595,052 views
Lecture 1: Introduction to Superposition
1:16:07
Lecture 1: Introduction to Superposition
MIT OpenCourseWare
8,105,100 views
Jeremy Utley: AI Ignites Human Creativity
59:59
Jeremy Utley: AI Ignites Human Creativity
NFFOnDemand
7,852 views
21. Chaos and Reductionism
1:37:33
21. Chaos and Reductionism
Stanford
1,774,017 views
15. Human Sexual Behavior I
1:41:43
15. Human Sexual Behavior I
Stanford
4,651,389 views
15 Most Listened To Classical Masterpieces of All Time. Beautiful classical music 🎼
2:14:39
15 Most Listened To Classical Masterpieces...
Classic Room
1,089,383 views
Think Fast, Talk Smart: Communication Techniques
58:20
Think Fast, Talk Smart: Communication Tech...
Stanford Graduate School of Business
47,513,611 views
Copyright © 2025. Made with ♥ in London by YTScribe.com