CRITICAL THINKING - Cognitive Biases: Anchoring [HD]

356.78k views847 WordsCopy TextShare
Wireless Philosophy
In this video, the cognitive scientist Laurie Santos (Yale University) explains the phenomenon of an...
Video Transcript:
(intro music) My name is Laurie Santos. I teach psychology at Yale University, and today I want to talk to you about anchoring. This lecture is part of a series on cognitive biases.
Let's do a math problem. really quickly, and you've gotta do it in your head Ready? First, multiply the following numbers: eight times seven times six times five times four times three times two times one.
OK, that's it. What's your guess? A thousand?
Two thousand? When the psychologists Danny Kahneman and Amos Tversky tried this with human subjects, subjects on average guessed about two thousand two hundred and fifty. Seems like an OK guess.
But now, let's suppose I gave you a different math problem. What if I gave you this one? Ready?
One times two times three times four times five times six times seven times eight. What's your answer? If you're like Kahneman and Tversky's subjects, your answer might be a bit different here.
For this question, their subjects guessed a lot lower. On average they said the answer was about five hundred and twelve. The first amazing thing about these similar mathematical estimates is that people get the answers really, really wrong.
In fact, the real answer? Well, for both, its forty thousand three hundred and twenty. People are off by an order of magnitude.
But the second, even more amazing thing is that people give different answers to the two problems, even though they're just different ways of asking exactly the same question. Why do we give completely different answers, when the same math problem is presented differently? The answer lies in how we make estimates.
When you have lots of time to do a math problem, like eight times seven times six times five times four times three times two times one, you can multiply all of the numbers together and get an exact product. But when you have to do the problem quickly, you don't really have time to finish. So you start with the first numbers.
You multiply eight times seven, and get fifty-six. And then you've gotta multiply that by six, and, well, you're guessing the final number's gotta be pretty big, bigger than fifty-six, like maybe two thousand or so. But when you do the second problem, you start with one times two, and, well, that's only two, and two times three's only six.
Your answer's gonna be pretty small, maybe only like five hundred or so. This process of guessing based on the first number you see is what's known as "anchoring. " The first number we think of when we do our estimate is the anchor.
And once we have an anchor in our head, well, we sort of adjust as needed from there. The problem is that our minds are biased not to adjust as much as we need to. The anchors are cognitively really strong.
In the first, problem you probably started with fifty-six, and then adjusted to an even bigger number from there. And in the second problem, you started with six, and then adjusted from there. The problem is that starting at different points leads to different final guesses.
Like real anchors, our estimated anchors kinda get us stuck in one spot. We often fail to drag the anchor far enough to get to a correct answer. Kahneman and Tversky discovered that this sort of anchoring bias happens all the time, even for anchors that are totally arbitrary.
For example, they asked people to spin a wheel with numbers from one to a hundred, and then asked them to estimate what percentage of countries in the United Nations are African. People who spun a ten on the wheel estimated that the number was about twenty-five percent. But people who spun a sixty-five estimated that the number was forty-five percent.
In another experiment, Dan Ariely and his colleagues had people write down the last two digits of their social security number. They were then asked whether they would pay that amount in dollars for a nice bottle of wine. Ariely and colleagues found that people in the highest quintile of social security numbers would pay three to four times as much for the exact same good.
Just setting up a larger anchor can make a person who would pay eight dollars for the bottle of wine be willing to spend twenty-seven dollars instead. Sadly for us, sales people use anchors against us all the time. How many times have you noticed a salesperson or an advertisement anchoring you to a particular price, or even to how much of a particular product you should buy?
Whether it's buying a car, or a sweater, or even renting a hotel room, our intuitions about what prices are reasonable to pay often come from some arbitrary anchor. So, the next time you're given an anchor, take a minute to think. Remember what happens when you drop your anger too high, and then consider thinking of a very different number.
It might affect your final estimate more than you expect. Subtitles by the Amara.
Related Videos
CRITICAL THINKING - Cognitive Biases: Alief [HD]
4:00
CRITICAL THINKING - Cognitive Biases: Alie...
Wireless Philosophy
147,730 views
CRITICAL THINKING - Fallacies: Ad Hominem [HD]
8:11
CRITICAL THINKING - Fallacies: Ad Hominem ...
Wireless Philosophy
435,830 views
Republic of Consciousness Prize 2024: The Candidates
10:11
Republic of Consciousness Prize 2024: The ...
Leaf by Leaf
1,171 views
"Why ignorance fails to recognize itself" Featuring David Dunning
22:21
"Why ignorance fails to recognize itself" ...
Macmillan Learning
350,475 views
Top Christmas Songs of All Time 🎅🏼 Best Christmas Music Playlist with Christmas Fireplace
Top Christmas Songs of All Time 🎅🏼 Best ...
Timeless Music
What Sentence Would You Give?: Watch for the Anchoring Bias
4:18
What Sentence Would You Give?: Watch for t...
Outsmarting Implicit Bias
13,013 views
CRITICAL THINKING - Fundamentals: Introduction to Critical Thinking [HD]
9:50
CRITICAL THINKING - Fundamentals: Introduc...
Wireless Philosophy
1,778,139 views
Beat Ronaldo, Win $1,000,000
22:45
Beat Ronaldo, Win $1,000,000
MrBeast
113,292,091 views
Think Fast, Talk Smart: Communication Techniques
58:20
Think Fast, Talk Smart: Communication Tech...
Stanford Graduate School of Business
42,957,547 views
1. Introduction to the Human Brain
1:19:56
1. Introduction to the Human Brain
MIT OpenCourseWare
11,859,114 views
The Dunning Kruger Effect
4:21
The Dunning Kruger Effect
Sprouts
2,844,973 views
Heuristics and biases in decision making, explained
3:49
Heuristics and biases in decision making, ...
Learn Liberty
622,299 views
Former FBI Agent Explains How to Read Body Language | Tradecraft | WIRED
14:44
Former FBI Agent Explains How to Read Body...
WIRED
53,083,952 views
Exposing Scientific Dogmas - Banned TED Talk - Rupert Sheldrake
17:32
Exposing Scientific Dogmas - Banned TED Ta...
After Skool
2,345,938 views
LEADERSHIP LAB: The Craft of Writing Effectively
1:21:52
LEADERSHIP LAB: The Craft of Writing Effec...
UChicago Social Sciences
8,516,505 views
CRITICAL THINKING - Cognitive Biases: Peak-End Effect [HD]
5:16
CRITICAL THINKING - Cognitive Biases: Peak...
Wireless Philosophy
94,077 views
How to Speak
1:03:43
How to Speak
MIT OpenCourseWare
19,877,071 views
Cognition - How Your Mind Can Amaze and Betray You: Crash Course Psychology #15
10:42
Cognition - How Your Mind Can Amaze and Be...
CrashCourse
2,926,440 views
Think Faster, Talk Smarter with Matt Abrahams
44:11
Think Faster, Talk Smarter with Matt Abrahams
Stanford Alumni
2,150,470 views
Steven Pinker: Linguistics as a Window to Understanding the Brain | Big Think
50:01
Steven Pinker: Linguistics as a Window to ...
Big Think
3,721,267 views
Copyright © 2025. Made with ♥ in London by YTScribe.com